Wednesday, September 29, 2010
Friday, September 24, 2010
iPost: Journal of Educational Psychology - Online First Publications & Volume 102, Issue 3
Journal of Educational Psychology - Online First Publications & Volume 102, Issue 3
APA Journal alerts for:
Journal of Educational Psychology
The following articles have been published online this week before they appear in a final print and online issue of Journal of Educational Psychology:
The effectiveness and relative importance of choice in the classroom.Patall, Erika A.; Cooper, Harris; Wynn, Susan R.doi: 10.1037/a0019545
The structure of academic self-concepts revisited: The nested marsh/shavelson model.Brunner, Martin; Keller, Ulrich; Dierendonck, Christophe; Reichert, Monique; Ugen, Sonja; Fischbach, Antoine; Martin, Romaindoi: 10.1037/a0019644
Sources of group and individual differences in emerging fraction skills.Hecht, Steven A.; Vagi, Kevin J.doi: 10.1037/a0019824
On the leaky math pipeline: Comparing implicit math-gender stereotypes and math withdrawal in female and male children and adolescents.Steffens, Melanie C.; Jelenec, Petra; Noack, Peterdoi: 10.1037/a0019920
Individual differences for self-regulating task-oriented reading activities.Vidal-Abarca, Eduardo; Mañá, Amelia; Gil, Lauradoi: 10.1037/a0020062
Accomplishment in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (stem) and its relation to stem educational dose: A 25-year longitudinal study.Wai, Jonathan; Lubinski, David; Benbow, Camilla P.; Steiger, James H.doi: 10.1037/a0019454
A comprehensive review of effect size reporting and interpreting practices in academic journals in education and psychology.Sun, Shuyan; Pan, Wei; Wang, Lihshing Leighdoi: 10.1037/a0019507
Is the link between large high schools and student victimization an illusion?Klein, Jennifer; Cornell, Deweydoi: 10.1037/a0019896
- A new issue is available:
Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring control–value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion.Page 531-549Pekrun, Reinhard; Goetz, Thomas; Daniels, Lia M.; Stupnisky, Robert H.; Perry, Raymond P.
Prediction of kindergartners' academic achievement from their effortful control and emotionality: Evidence for direct and moderated relations.Page 550-560Valiente, Carlos; Lemery-Chalfant, Kathryn; Swanson, Jodi
Should students have a gap year? Motivation and performance factors relevant to time out after completing school.Page 561-576Martin, Andrew J.
Social contagion of motivation between teacher and student: Analyzing underlying processes.Page 577-587Radel, Rémi; Sarrazin, Philippe; Legrain, Pascal; Wild, T. Cameron
Engaging students in learning activities: It is not autonomy support or structure but autonomy support and structure.Page 588-600Jang, Hyungshim; Reeve, Johnmarshall; Deci, Edward L.
Helping students soar to success on computers: An investigation of the SOAR study method for computer-based learning.Page 601-614Jairam, Dharmananda; Kiewra, Kenneth A.
Does language matter in multimedia learning? Personalization principle revisited.Page 615-624Kartal, Günizi
A is for apple: Mnemonic symbols hinder the interpretation of algebraic expressions.Page 625-634McNeil, Nicole M.; Weinberg, Aaron; Hattikudur, Shanta; Stephens, Ana C.; Asquith, Pamela; Knuth, Eric J.; Alibali, Martha W.
Listening comprehension, oral expression, reading comprehension, and written expression: Related yet unique language systems in grades 1, 3, 5, and 7.Page 635-651Berninger, Virginia W.; Abbott, Robert D.
Does growth rate in oral reading fluency matter in predicting reading comprehension achievement?Page 652-667Kim, Young-Suk; Petscher, Yaacov; Schatschneider, Christopher; Foorman, Barbara
Reading achievement across three language groups: Growth estimates for overall reading and reading subskills obtained with the Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey.Page 668-686Roberts, Greg; Mohammed, Sarojani S.; Vaughn, Sharon
Reading comprehension of scientific text: A domain-specific test of the direct and inferential mediation model of reading comprehension.Page 687-700Cromley, Jennifer G.; Snyder-Hogan, Lindsey E.; Luciw-Dubas, Ulana A.
Predictors of reading comprehension for struggling readers: The case of Spanish-speaking language minority learners.Page 701-711Mancilla-Martinez, Jeannette; Lesaux, Nonie K.
Effects of cross-language transfer on first-language phonological awareness and literacy skills in Chinese children receiving English instruction.Page 712-728Chen, Xi; Xu, Fen; Nguyen, Thien-Kim; Hong, Guanglei; Wang, Yun
Does math self-efficacy mediate the effect of the perceived classroom environment on standardized math test performance?Page 729-740Fast, Lisa A.; Lewis, James L.; Bryant, Michael J.; Bocian, Kathleen A.; Cardullo, Richard A.; Rettig, Michael; Hammond, Kimberly A.
Effects on teachers' self-efficacy and job satisfaction: Teacher gender, years of experience, and job stress.Page 741-756Klassen, Robert M.; Chiu, Ming Ming
African Americans and boys: Understanding the literacy gap, tracing academic trajectories, and evaluating the role of learning-related skills.Page 757-771Matthews, J. S.; Kizzie, Karmen T.; Rowley, Stephanie J.; Cortina, Kai
iPost: Neuropsychology - Online First Publications & Volume 24, Issue 5
Neuropsychology - Online First Publications & Volume 24, Issue 5
APA Journal alerts for:
Neuropsychology
The following articles have been published online this week before they appear in a final print and online issue of Neuropsychology:
The independent influence of apathy and depression on cognitive functioning in parkinson's disease.Butterfield, London C.; Cimino, Cynthia R.; Oelke, Lynn E.; Hauser, Robert A.; Sanchez-Ramos, Juandoi: 10.1037/a0019650
Intraindividual variability in reaction time predicts cognitive outcomes 5 years later.Bielak, Allison A. M.; Hultsch, David F.; Strauss, Esther; MacDonald, Stuart W. S.; Hunter, Michael A.doi: 10.1037/a0019802
Is decision making really impaired in eating disorders?Guillaume, Sébastien; Sang, Caroline Ngo Ton; Jaussent, Isabelle; Raingeard, Isabelle; Bringer, Jacques; Jollant, Fabrice; Courtet, Philippedoi: 10.1037/a0019806
- A new issue is available:
Testing covariates of Type 2 diabetes-cognition associations in older adults: Moderating or mediating effects?Page 547-562McFall, G. Peggy; Geall, Bonnie P.; Fischer, Ashley L.; Dolcos, Sanda; Dixon, Roger A.
Influence of age on practice effects in longitudinal neurocognitive change.Page 563-572Salthouse, Timothy A.
Relationship between depression, fatigue, subjective cognitive impairment, and objective neuropsychological functioning in patients with multiple sclerosis.Page 573-580Kinsinger, Sarah W.; Lattie, Emily; Mohr, David C.
The nature of naming errors in primary progressive aphasia versus acute post-stroke aphasia.Page 581-589Budd, Maggi A.; Kortte, Kathleen; Cloutman, Lauren; Newhart, Melissa; Gottesman, Rebecca F.; Davis, Cameron; Heidler-Gary, Jennifer; Seay, Margaret W.; Hillis, Argye E.
Executive function in individuals with subthreshold autism traits.Page 590-598Christ, Shawn E.; Kanne, Stephen M.; Reiersen, Angela M.
Recognition by familiarity is preserved in Parkinson's without dementia and Lewy-Body disease.Page 599-607Algarabel, Salvador; Rodríguez, Lucía-Azahara; Escudero, Joaquín; Fuentes, Manuel; Peset, Vicente; Pitarque, Alfonso; Cómbita, Lina-Marcela; Mazón, Jose F.
Scene construction in schizophrenia.Page 608-615Raffard, Stéphane; D'Argembeau, Arnaud; Bayard, Sophie; Boulenger, Jean-Philippe; Van der Linden, Martial
Age-related practice effects across longitudinal neuropsychological assessments in older people with schizophrenia.Page 616-624Granholm, Eric; Link, Peter; Fish, Scott; Kraemer, Helena; Jeste, Dilip
Data-driven methodology illustrating mechanisms underlying word list recall: Applications to clinical research.Page 625-636Longenecker, Julia; Kohn, Philip; Liu, Stanley; Zoltick, Brad; Weinberger, Daniel R.; Elvevåg, Brita
Appraising the ANT: Psychometric and theoretical considerations of the Attention Network Test.Page 637-651MacLeod, Jeffrey W.; Lawrence, Michael A.; McConnell, Meghan M.; Eskes, Gail A.; Klein, Raymond M.; Shore, David I.
Modeling the effects of hypoglycemia on a two-choice task in adult humans.Page 652-660Geddes, Jacqueline; Ratcliff, Roger; Allerhand, Michael; Childers, Russ; Wright, Rohana J.; Frier, Brian M.; Deary, Ian J.
Laterality effects for musical structure processing: A dichotic listening study.Page 661-666Hoch, Lisianne; Tillmann, Barbara
A pilot study of the neuropsychological benefits of computerized cognitive rehabilitation in Ugandan children with HIV.Page 667-673Boivin, Michael J.; Busman, Rachelle A.; Parikh, Sujal M.; Bangirana, Paul; Page, Connie F.; Opoka, Robert O.; Giordani, Bruno
The role of vision in the Corsi Block-Tapping task: Evidence from blind and sighted people.Page 674-679Ruggiero, Gennaro; Iachini, Tina
Sunday, September 19, 2010
On the road again--blogging lite Sept 20 thru Oct 4
I will be on the road from Sept 20 thru Oct 4th. I will be traveling abroad making a variety of professional presentations "down under" (put some shrimp on the barbie for me)
I don't expect much time to blog...except for possible "push" type FYI posts re: content posted at other blogs.....or...mobile blogging (iPosts: check out the link.....it is very cool...but, of course, I tend to be a tech nerd)......
I shall return.
Technorati Tags: psychology, IQs Corner, Intelligence, travel
iPost: Gv training study eliminates Gv spatial gender gap
Original Tweet: http://api.twitter.com/1/NeuropathLrng/status/24780391565
Sent via TweetDeck (www.tweetdeck.com)
Sent from KMcGrew iPhone (IQMobile). (If message includes an image-double click on it to make larger-if hard to see)
iPost: Prefrontal cortex linked introspection
#neuroscience #science
Original Tweet: http://api.twitter.com/1/NeuroscienceNew/status/24799102263
Sent via TweetDeck (www.tweetdeck.com)
Sent from KMcGrew iPhone (IQMobile). (If message includes an image-double click on it to make larger-if hard to see)
Saturday, September 18, 2010
Research Bytes 9-18-2010: Gf and math longitudinal study
Abstract
The association between fluid intelligence and inter-individual differences was investigated using multilevel growth curve modeling applied to data measuring intra-individual improvement on math achievement tests. A sample of 166 students (88 boys and 78 girls), ranging in age from 11 to 14 (M = 12.3, SD = 0.64), was tested. These individuals took four math achievement tests, which were vertically equated via Item Response Theory, at the beginning and end of the seventh and eighth grade. The cognitive abilities studied were Numerical Reasoning, Abstract Reasoning, Verbal Reasoning, and Spatial Reasoning (as measured by the Differential Reasoning Test). The general cognitive factor was significantly associated with the parameters of initial level (intercept) and rate of change (slope). A high level of intelligence was associated with higher initial scores, as well as a steeper rise in math scores across the two years.
Keywords: Growth curve modeling; Intelligence; Math learning; Multilevel analysis
Technorati Tags: Psychology, school psychology, developmental psychology, educational psychology, forensic psychology, neuropsychology, special education, intelligence, cognitive abilities, cognition, intelligence theories, CHC theory, CHC, Cattell-Horn-Carroll, intelligence, cognition, IQ, IQ tests, Gf, Gq, math, math LD, dyscalculia
Tuesday, September 14, 2010
CHC IQ test "Periodic Table of Cognitive Elements" is BACK!!!! WAIS-IV example
Below are a set of slides that include the new periodic table of cognitive elements and its use in a visual-graphic CHC summary of the WAIS-IV. Stay tuned...more of these are in the works.
The IAP AP 101 WAIS-IV report link included in one of the slides can also be accessed by clicking here.
Images can be enlarged by double clicking on them. Enjoy.
iPost: Higher Gv in females with male co-twin
Original Tweet: http://api.twitter.com/1/PsychScience/status/24474465621
Sent via TweetDeck (www.tweetdeck.com)
Sent from KMcGrew iPhone (IQMobile). (If message includes an image-double click on it to make larger-if hard to see)
iPost: Special JPA issue on the Flynn effect
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment Online Table of Contents Alert
- A new issue of Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment is available online:
- 1 October 2010; Vol. 28, No. 5
- The below Table of Contents is available online at: http://jpa.sagepub.com/content/vol28/issue5/?etoc
Articles
- Guest Editors' Introduction to the Special Issue of JPA on the Flynn Effect
- Alan S. Kaufman and Lawrence G. Weiss
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 379-381
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/reprint/28/5/379
- "In What Way Are Apples and Oranges Alike?" A Critique of Flynn's Interpretation of the Flynn Effect
- Alan S. Kaufman
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 382-398
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/382
- Peeking Inside the "Black Box" of the Flynn Effect: Evidence From Three Wechsler Instruments
- Xiaobin Zhou, Jianjun Zhu, and Lawrence G. Weiss
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 399-411
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/399
- Problems With IQ Gains: The Huge Vocabulary Gap
- James R. Flynn
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 412-433
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/412
- The Flynn Effect: So What?
- Robert J. Sternberg
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 434-440
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/434
- "Apples and Oranges Are Both Round": Furthering the Discussion on the Flynn Effect
- Stephen J. Ceci and Tomoe Kanaya
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 441-447
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/441
- The Flynn Effect and Its Critics: Rusty Linchpins and "Lookin' for g and Gf in Some of the Wrong Places"
- Kevin S. McGrew
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 448-468
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/448
- IQ Scores Should Be Corrected for the Flynn Effect in High-Stakes Decisions
- Jack M. Fletcher, Karla K. Stuebing, and Lisa C. Hughes
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 469-473
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/469
- IQ Scores Should Not Be Adjusted for the Flynn Effect in Capital Punishment Cases
- Leigh D. Hagan, Eric Y. Drogin, and Thomas J. Guilmette
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 474-476
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/474
- Failure to Apply the Flynn Correction in Death Penalty Litigation: Standard Practice of Today Maybe, but Certainly Malpractice of Tomorrow
- Cecil R. Reynolds, John Niland, John E. Wright, and Michal Rosenn
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 477-481
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/477
- Considerations on the Flynn Effect
- Lawrence G. Weiss
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 482-493
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/482
- Looking Through Flynn's Rose-Colored Scientific Spectacles
- Alan S. Kaufman
- Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 2010;28 494-505
http://jpa.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/28/5/494
Monday, September 13, 2010
Dissertation Dish: Brief neuropsych. battery for reading disability screening
Abstract
Identification of reading disorder in children is the first step in providing appropriate intervention. Neuropsychological assessment provides comprehensive information regarding cognitive strengths and weaknesses; however, there are several inherent drawbacks to this type of evaluation that limit its accessibility, including the time and cost involved. The objective of this study was to determine whether an abbreviated neuropsychological battery, decreasing both time and cost could effectively identify reading disorder. 78 children, ages 6-18, were administered the Woodcock Johnson Academic Achievement Scales, Third Edition (WJ-III) Broad Reading Index, the California Verbal Learning Test for Children (CVLT-C), the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF), and the Matrix Reasoning, Similarities, Block Design, and Vocabulary subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). Of these 78 children 52 had been previously diagnosed with reading disorder, while the remaining 26 had no diagnosis. A discriminant function analysis resulted in a significant Wilks' Lambda and the abbreviated battery successfully classified cases into their correct diagnostic groups at a rate of 80%. Two subsequent analyses were computed, first with the combination of the WJ-III, the BRIEF, and Similarities, and then with just the WJ-III. Results of these analyses indicated that both models significantly discriminated between groups. The three variable model predicted diagnostic classification with 78% accuracy, while the single variable model predicted with 65% accuracy. Results of this study suggest that an abbreviated battery can have diagnostic utility in screening for reading disorder
Technorati Tags: Psychology, school psychology, developmental psychology, educational psychology, forensic psychology, neuropsychology, special education, intelligence, cognitive abilities, cognition, intelligence theories, CHC theory, CHC, Cattell-Horn-Carroll, intelligence, cognition, IQ, IQ tests, Gf, Gc, Gv, Ga, Gsm, Glr, Gs, Gq, Grw, WJ-R, WJ III, Woodcock-Johnson, WJ III NU, reading disability, screening, LD, learning disabilities, brief neuropsych, dissertation dish
IQs Corner Recent Literature of Interest 09-13-10
Information regarding this feature, its basis, and the reasons for type of references included in each weekly installment can be found in a prior post.
Technorati Tags: psychology, school psychology, educational psychology, neuropsychology, research, recent literature, IQs Corner
Friday, September 10, 2010
iPost: Psychological Review - Online First Publications & Volume 117, Issue 3
Psychological Review - Online First Publications & Volume 117, Issue 3
APA Journal alerts for:
Psychological Review
The following articles have been published online this week before they appear in a final print and online issue of Psychological Review:
Neurally constrained modeling of perceptual decision making.Purcell, Braden A.; Heitz, Richard P.; Cohen, Jeremiah Y.; Schall, Jeffrey D.; Logan, Gordon D.; Palmeri, Thomas J.doi: 10.1037/a0020311
The robust beauty of ordinary information.Katsikopoulos, Konstantinos V.; Schooler, Lael J.; Hertwig, Ralphdoi: 10.1037/a0020418
The categorization-individuation model: An integrative account of the other-race recognition deficit.Hugenberg, Kurt; Young, Steven G.; Bernstein, Michael J.; Sacco, Donald F.doi: 10.1037/a0020463
Democracy under uncertainty: The wisdom of crowds and the free-rider problem in group decision making.Kameda, Tatsuya; Tsukasaki, Takafumi; Hastie, Reid; Berg, Nathandoi: 10.1037/a0020699
A new issue is available:
A dual-stage two-phase model of selective attention.Page 759-784Hübner, Ronald; Steinhauser, Marco; Lehle, Carola
Rationales for indirect speech: The theory of the strategic speaker.Page 785-807Lee, James J.; Pinker, Steven
A biologically realistic cortical model of eye movement control in reading.Page 808-830Heinzle, Jakob; Hepp, Klaus; Martin, Kevan A. C.
Assessing the belief bias effect with ROCs: It's a response bias effect.Page 831-863Dube, Chad; Rotello, Caren M.; Heit, Evan
Two-stage dynamic signal detection: A theory of choice, decision time, and confidence.Page 864-901Pleskac, Timothy J.; Busemeyer, Jerome R.
Conscious thought is for facilitating social and cultural interactions: How mental simulations serve the animal–culture interface.Page 945-971Baumeister, Roy F.; Masicampo, E. J.
Incubation, insight, and creative problem solving: A unified theory and a connectionist model.Page 994-1024Hélie, Sébastien; Sun, Ron
iPost: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition - Online First Publications & Volume 36, Issue 5
APA Journal alerts for:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
The following articles have been published online this week before they appear in a final print and online issue of Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition:
Attention is required for acquisition but not expression of new response biases.Miles, James D.; Proctor, Robert W.doi: 10.1037/a0020603
Metacognitive control and spaced practice: Clarifying what people do and why.Toppino, Thomas C.; Cohen, Michael S.doi: 10.1037/a0020949
A new issue is available:
Spinning in the scanner: Neural correlates of virtual reorientation.Page 1097-1107Sutton, Jennifer E.; Joanisse, Marc F.; Newcombe, Nora S.
Tracking the time course of orthographic information in spoken-word recognition.Page 1108-1117Salverda, Anne Pier; Tanenhaus, Michael K.
Repeated testing produces superior transfer of learning relative to repeated studying.Page 1118-1133Butler, Andrew C.
Loss of cognitive skill across delays: Constraints for theories of cognitive skill acquisition.Page 1134-1149Wilkins, Nicolas J.; Rawson, Katherine A.
Semantic preview benefit in eye movements during reading: A parafoveal fast-priming study.Page 1150-1170Hohenstein, Sven; Laubrock, Jochen; Kliegl, Reinhold
Causal status and coherence in causal-based categorization.Page 1171-1206Rehder, Bob; Kim, ShinWoo
Examining the relationship between free recall and immediate serial recall: The effects of list length and output order.Page 1207-1241Ward, Geoff; Tan, Lydia; Grenfell-Essam, Rachel
Mental subtraction in high- and lower skilled arithmetic problem solvers: Verbal report versus operand-recognition paradigms.Page 1242-1255Thevenot, Catherine; Castel, Caroline; Fanget, Muriel; Fayol, Michel
Embedded words in visual word recognition: Does the left hemisphere see the rain in brain?Page 1256-1266McCormick, Samantha F.; Davis, Colin J.; Brysbaert, Marc
Making things difficult in lexical decision: The impact of pseudohomophones and transposed-letter nonwords on frequency and semantic priming effects.Page 1267-1289Lupker, Stephen J.; Pexman, Penny M.
Information-processing alternatives to holistic perception: Identifying the mechanisms of secondary-level holism within a categorization paradigm.Page 1290-1313Fifić, Mario; Townsend, James T.
Imagined positive emotions and inhibitory control: The differentiated effect of pride versus happiness.Page 1314-1320Katzir, Maayan; Eyal, Tal; Meiran, Nachshon; Kessler, Yoav
Revisiting the novelty effect: When familiarity, not novelty, enhances memory.Page 1321-1330Poppenk, J.; Köhler, S.; Moscovitch, M.
False memories seconds later: The rapid and compelling onset of illusory recognition.Page 1331-1338Flegal, Kristin E.; Atkins, Alexandra S.; Reuter-Lorenz, Patricia A.
Long-lasting effects of briefly flashed words and pseudowords in ultrarapid serial visual presentation.Page 1339-1345Albrecht, Thorsten; Vorberg, Dirk
Item-specific encoding produces an additional benefit of directed forgetting: Evidence from intrusion errors.Page 1346-1354Sahakyan, Lili; Delaney, Peter F.
iPost: Ga sensitivity, speech perception and reading
http://www.springerlink.com/content/44202445tm731642/
Sent from KMcGrew iPhone (IQMobile). (If message includes an image-double click on it to make larger-if hard to see)
iPost: Gv and visualization learning meta-analysis
http://www.springerlink.com/content/m75728122768j6x4/
Sent from KMcGrew iPhone (IQMobile). (If message includes an image-double click on it to make larger-if hard to see)
Re: Understanding the positive role of neighborhood socioeconomic advantage in achievement: The contribution of the home, child care, and school environments.
Earthlink wrote:
Article info at link below. An example why controlling for community SES is important in national test standardizations.
Sent from KMcGrew iPhone (IQMobile). (If message includes an image-double click on it to make larger-if hard to see)
iPost: A general multilevel SEM framework for assessing multilevel mediation.
iPost: Seeing the forest for the trees: Prevalence of low scores on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, fourth edition (WISC-IV).
http://feedproxy.google.com/~r/apa-journals-pas/~3/VhXyYLY9LPI/650
Sent from KMcGrew iPhone (IQMobile). (If message includes an image-double click on it to make larger-if hard to see)
Understanding the positive role of neighborhood socioeconomic advantage in achievement: The contribution of the home, child care, and school environments.
Article info at link below. An example why controlling for community SES is important in national test standardizations.
iPost: What counts in the development of young children's number knowledge?
http://feedproxy.google.com/~r/apa-journals-dev/~3/iTTerfkirio/1309
Sent from KMcGrew iPhone (IQMobile). (If message includes an image-double click on it to make larger-if hard to see)
Monday, September 06, 2010
iPost: A Controlled Trial of Herbal Treatment for ADHD
Sent from KMcGrew iPhone (IQMobile). (If message includes an image-double click on it to make larger-if hard to see)
Thursday, September 02, 2010
Williams guest blog comments on Scott Barry Kaufman guest post on Flynn effect and IQ disparities
Bob Williams states:
I would like to offer some detailed comments:
Literacy involves the ability to write, read, and comprehend information of varying levels of complexity. It is estimated that there are 774 million illiterate adults in the world, 65% whom are women (UNESCO Intsistute for Statistics, 2007). In the United States alone, 5% of the adult population is completely nonliterate (Kirsch, Jungeblut, Jenkins, & Kolstad, 1993).
Literacy is usually referenced as a binary condition, not as degrees. But the discussion here suggests that it is to be both considered as binary and as a continuous variable. Literacy should follow the same path as education in its relationship to IQ, namely that educational achievement flows from IQ and not vice versa. I will discuss this further below.
One study showed that the IQ and literacy scores of Blacks increased in parallel from 1980 to 2000 (Dickens & Flynn, 2006). Murray showed essentially no change over the period 1980 to 1990. C. Murray/Intelligence, 35 (2007) 305–318
The importance of being able to read for performance on an IQ test cannot be understated. IQ tests must be used as intended. If the test involves reading, the testee must be able to read. Professionals are unlikely to violate this common sense requirement. There are, however, high quality IQ tests that do not require reading, either of instructions or test items. The Raven's set is the best known example, but other tests designed for children and the illiterate are also available (the Kohs and DAM are examples).
Instead of measuring ‘intelligence' in a nonliterate test-taker, the test is measuring that person's inability to read. This happens only when the test is administered by someone who cannot follow instructions or has an agenda and wishes to collect bad measurements. These findings have led some researchers to propose that such IQ gaps found across ethnicities, races, and nationalities suggests a difference in innate brain capacity (see Lynn & Vanhanen, 2006).
Besides the findings you mentioned, there is a wealth of data showing that intelligence is determined by biological, not social factors. Much of The g Factor was devoted to demonstrating this. The heritability of IQ consistently shows up above 80% in adults and can be determined by diverse methods (Falconer's formula, twin correlation, path analysis, and by subtracting the environmental effect from 1.0).
If increasing literacy were really explaining a number of seemingly different IQ trends, then you would expect to see a few things. First, within a population you should expect increased education of literacy skills to be associated with an increase in the average IQ of that population. One of the important studies of the Flynn Effect (FE) was conducted by Nettelbeck and Wilson [Intelligence 32 (2004) 85–93]. The controls in their study were extraordinary. With virtually every variable held constant, even to the people who did the measurements and their instrumentation, they found a FE, but no change in inspection times (IT). I asked Nettlebeck if there could have been any nutritional or social changes between his two study groups (separated by 20 years). He said emphatically that there was not.
There is also a problem with focusing on literacy because it requires time to develop or not develop, yet IQ can be measured in young children and even infants. Those early measurements are predictive of adult IQ and educational attainment. [See Fagan's work with infants.] And this:
"Within the United States, the mean Black–White group difference in IQ has not changed significantly over the past 100 years despite significant improvements in the conditions of Black Americans. The same magnitude of difference is observed as early as age 2 1/2 years." [Rushton, J.P. and Jensen, A.R. (2005). Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, Vol. 11, No. 2, 235-294.]
Second, IQ gains should be most pronounced in the lower half of the IQ bell curve since this is the section of the population that prior to the education would have obtained relatively lower scores due to their inability to comprehend the intelligence test's instructions. As you know, this has been reported in some studies and not found in others. As Must and Must have shown, the FE is not invariant, at least in Estonia. There is little reason to expect that it is invariant elsewhere. The Nettelbeck study, for example, did not report this finding, nor would it have been expected in such an otherwise homogeneous group, but the FE was still present.
If all these predictions hold up, there would be support for the notion that secular IQ gains and race differences are not different phenomena but have a common origin in literacy. Not likely. The differences between racial groups are g loaded. Here is the short section 5 (conclusions) from a recent paper by Rushton and Jensen:
"Heritable g is at the core of the debate over how much the mean Black–White gap in IQ and school achievement is due to the genes rather than to the environment, and therefore, how much it can be expected to narrow. While g and genetic estimates correlate significantly positively with Black–White differences 0.61 and 0.48 (P < 0.001), they correlate significantly negatively (or not at all) with the secular gains (r = -0.33; P < 0.001) and 0.13 (ns). Similarly, g loadings and heritabilities from the items of the Raven Matrices correlate significantly positively with each other and with Black–White differences (mean r = 0.74, P < 0.01). Although the secular gains are on g-loaded tests (such as the Wechsler), they are negatively correlated with the most g-loaded components of those tests. Tests lose their g loadedness over time as the result of training, retesting, and familiarity (te Nijenhuis et al., 2007).
Some issues, however, remain to be resolved. For example, Lynn (2009) found a secular rise in the Developmental Quotients of infants in the first two years of life, which he suggested was due to improved pre-natal and early post-natal nutrition. He supported his conjecture by pointing to equivalent gains in birth weight, stature, and brain size, and the correlation of these variables with later IQ. If it becomes possible to disentangle environmental factors that do affect g, from the environmental factors that do not affect g, the negative correlation between g and secular gains may increase from -0.33 to nearer - 1.00.
Predictions about the Black–White IQ gap narrowing due to the secular rise is based on faith rather than evidence. There is no more reason to expect Black–White differences in IQ to narrow as a result of the secular rise in IQ than to expect male–female differences in height to narrow as a result of the secular rise in height. The (mostly heritable) cause of the one is not the (mostly environmental) cause of the other. From the present perspective, the Flynn Effect (the secular rise in IQ) is not a Jensen Effect (because it does not occur on g).."
[Rushton and Jensen, Intelligence, Volume 38, Issue 2, March-April 2010, Pages 213-219 ]
To test these predictions, Marks looked at samples representative of whole populations (rather than individuals), and used ecological methods to calculate statistical associations between IQ and literacy rates across different countries.
The problem is that he is looking at a consequence of intelligence that has little, if anything, to do with causation. But Marks made it clear that he thinks he is looking at the cause and not the effect. Here is a quote from the paper you referenced:
"Secondly, the differences in IQ scores that exist today between different populations are artifacts of large, confounding literacy differences that exist between these populations. Thirdly, white-black differences in IQ scores are caused by literacy differences between these racial groups. Comparing the average IQ test scores of racial groups or populations without controlling for literacy is illegitimate."
None of the above is correct. The population group differences have been shown to be highly heritable biological differences and are not the result of social factors. The race difference shows up in testing at age 2-1/2 and in the robust correlate, head size, at birth. Literacy is not a birth nor an early childhood parameter. Marks is off the mark.
It should also be noted that Mark's findings only speak to populations (not individuals) and do not say much about causation. Marks made a very clear comment (see above) about his view of causation. It seems to me that anyone wishing to show that literacy is a factor in IQ test scores would use structural equation modeling to evaluate the obvious alternatives, with the goal of showing the one that best fits available data.
variables that affect both literacy and IQ. Still, the result that population level literacy changes with population IQ is suggestive that increased literacy is causing increased IQ.
No, it is not. It instead suggests that IQ is accompanied by literacy. It is well known that years of education correlates positively with IQ and to such an extent that educational attainment can be used as a rough proxy for IQ. But people do not become more intelligent as they spend more years in school. Education is not intelligence, but rather acts as a tool set for the application of intelligence. IQ tends to be stable over most of our lives, after about age 6, but even before that age, it is highly predictive of later measurements and is predictive of educational achievement. For a second time let me suggest Fagan's paper: The prediction, from infancy, of adult IQ and achievement Intelligence, Volume 35, Issue 3, May-June 2007, Pages 225-231
Joseph F. Fagan, Cynthia R. Holland, Karyn Wheeler
Marks did just that by scanning the literature for datasets containing test estimates for populations of groups taking both the Armed Forces Qualifications Test and tests of literacy. One study on nine groups of soldiers differing in job and reading ability found a correlation of .96 between the Armed Forces Qualifications Test and reading achievement (Sticht, Caylor, Kern, & Fox, 1972).
Yes, because reading is highly g loaded. Jensen devoted a long discussion to this in The g Factor (pages 279-282). Reading does not boost g; it is g that makes reading comprehension high or low as a function of individual intelligence.
Another study obtained reading scores for 17-year olds for those same ethnic groups and dates and found a correlation of .997 between reading scores and Armed Forces Qualifications Test scores (Campbell et al., 2000). This nearly perfect correlation was based on six pairs of data points from six independent population samples evaluated by two separate groups of investigators.
These "nearly perfect correlations" are the subject of a very well done paper in Intelligence: The issue of power in the identification of “g” with lower-order factors, Pages 336-344, Dora Matzke, Conor V. Dolan, Dylan Molenaar
They commented in the conclusions section, "Our examination of published studies revealed that most of our case studies, which reported a perfect correlation between g and a lower-order factor, were underpowered, with power coefficient rarely exceeding 0.3." The comment above strikes me as precisely the kind of underpowered case Matzke et al. were addressing.
"On the basis of the studies summarized here, there can be little doubt that the Armed Forces Qualifications Test is a measure of literacy."
There is plenty of evidence presented in The Bell Curve that the AFQT is heavily g loaded. There is one g and it doesn't matter how it is measured, the g is still the same thing. IQ tests get virtually all of their validity by acting as a proxy for g. If g is measured by the Raven's, the WJ-III, or a battery of reaction time tests, the thing at the root is the same g. The entire argument of literacy should be immediately questioned when it is used in connection with the FE, since it has been clearly shown that the largest secular gains have been observed in abstract test items. The Raven's shows this quite well. I once had about a one hour chat with John Raven and asked him if he thought the gains in the Raven's tests were g loaded. He only replied that the scores were increasing. When I asked Jim Flynn if the FE gains were g loaded, he gave me his usual historical (Rabbit and Hound story) answer, then said "I don't know." Most studies that have attempted to determine the g loading of the FE have found no loading. Rushton and Jensen have made this point as have Must and Must and others.
In The g Factor, Jensen showed that a positive FE could be shown for abstract test items, while a negative FE could be shown for scholastic items (see page 322). It is more than odd that the latter (literacy) would contribute in the opposite direction along with abstract items.
Potential research avenues to be explored.
The Marks study suggests a crucial environmental factor is literacy. Marks has tried to make that point, but he has skipped the necessary details and has ignored the strong biological effects as well as the early age factor that I have previously pointed out. If this is so, then interventions that increase literacy will also narrow the IQ gap found between different races and nationalities. To the best of my knowledge, no social or training factor has ever been shown to cause a real and permanent increase in g. Training increases the Spearman s loading and lowers the g loading. It does not improve real intelligence (g).
This latest research on the environmental effects of nutrition (Colom et al., 2005, but see Flynn, 2009), disease, literacy, and more on both the rise in IQ and ethnic, racial, and national disparities in IQ point to the importance of the environment for developing intelligence as well as the importance for researchers to be very careful when they use intelligence test performance (especially verbal tests) to make inferences about hereditary differences between different ethnic groups and nationalities.
Environmental factors account for about 17% of the variance in intelligence in developed nations. It must be more in undeveloped nations, but environmental factors act through biological mechanisms (toxins, disease, etc., not social factors) and, so far, these have all been negative. Ethnic group IQs are highly heritable as is evident by such displays as regression to the mean (for the group), inbreeding depression, twin studies, and adoption studies. Marks failed to explain the finding that adopted children reach adult IQs that are as predicted by their biological peers and those IQs are uncorrelated with their adoptive siblings. You would expect similar literacy within a family, but these studies show that only heritability was able to explain the outcomes.
Bob Williams
Technorati Tags: psychology, educational psychology, intelligence, Flynn effect, race and IQ, ethnicity and IQ, national IQs, guest blog post, Bob Williams, Scott Barry Kaufman