Available online 6 January 2016
Beyond Born versus Made: A New Look at Expertise
- Available online 6 January 2016
Abstract
Why
are some people so much more successful than other people in music,
sports, games, business, and other complex domains? This question is the
subject of one of psychology's oldest debates. Over 20 years ago,
Ericsson, Krampe, and Tesch-Römer (1993) proposed that individual
differences in performance in domains such as these largely reflect
accumulated amount of “deliberate practice.” More controversially,
making exceptions only for height and body size, Ericsson et al.
explicitly rejected any direct role for innate factors (“talent”) in the
attainment of expert performance. This view has since become the
dominant theoretical account of expertise and has filtered into the
popular imagination through books such as Malcolm Gladwell's (2008) Outliers.
Nevertheless, as we discuss in this chapter, evidence from recent
research converges on the conclusion that this view is not defensible.
Recent meta-analyses have demonstrated that although deliberate practice
accounts for a sizeable proportion of the variance in performance in
complex domains, it consistently leaves an even larger proportion of the
variance unexplained and potentially explainable by other factors. In
light of this evidence, we offer a “new look” at expertise that takes
into account a wide range of factors.
Keywords
- Cognitive ability;
- Deliberate practice;
- Expert performance;
- Expertise;
- Genetics;
- Individual differences;
- Intelligence;
- Skilled performance;
- Talent
1. Introduction
No
one can deny that some people are vastly more skilled than other people
in certain domains. Consider that the winning time for the New York
City Marathon in 2014—just under 2 h and 11 min—was more than 2 h better than the average finishing time (http://www.tcsnycmarathon.org/results). Or consider that Jonas von Essen, en route to winning the 2014 World Memory Championships, memorized 26 decks of cards in an hour (http://www.world-memory-statistics.com).
What are the origins of this striking variability in human expertise?1
Why are some people so much better at certain tasks than other people?
One particularly influential theoretical account attempts to explain
individual differences in expertise in terms of deliberate practice (e.g., Boot and Ericsson, 2013, Ericsson, 2007, Ericsson et al., 1993, Ericsson et al., 2005 and Keith and Ericsson, 2007).
Here, we describe the mounting evidence that challenges this view. This
evidence converges on the conclusion that deliberate practice is an
important piece of the expertise puzzle, but not the only piece, or even
necessarily the largest piece. In light of this evidence, we offer a
“new look” at expertise that takes into account a wide range of factors,
including those known to be substantially heritable.
The rest of the chapter is organized into the following sections. We describe the deliberate practice view (Section 2) and then review evidence that challenges it (Section 3).
Then, we review evidence for factors other than deliberate practice
that may also account for individual differences in expertise (Section 4). We then describe an integrative approach to research on expertise (Section 5). Finally, we summarize our major findings and comment on directions for future research (Section 6).
2. The Deliberate Practice View
The
question of what explains individual differences in expertise is the
topic of one of psychology's oldest debates. One view is that experts
are “born.” This view holds that although training is necessary to
become an expert, innate ability—talent—limits the ultimate level of performance that a person can achieve in a domain. Nearly 150 years ago, in his book Hereditary Genius, Francis Galton (1869)
argued for this view based on his finding that eminence in domains such
as music, science, literature, and art tends to run in families, going
so far as to conclude that “social hindrances cannot impede men of high
ability, from becoming eminent [and] social advantages are incompetent
to give that status, to a man of moderate ability” (p. 41). The opposing
view is that experts are “made.” This view argues that if talent exists
at all, its effects are overshadowed by training. John Watson (1930),
the founder of behaviorism, championed this view when he guaranteed
that he could take any infant at random and train him to become “any
type of specialist [he] might select...regardless of his talents” (p.
104).
The modern era of scientific research on expertise traces back to the 1940s and the research of the Dutch psychologist Adriaan de Groot (1946/1978).
Himself an internationally competitive chess player, de Groot
investigated the thought processes underlying chess expertise using a
“choice-of-move” paradigm in which he gave chess players chess positions
and instructed them to verbalize their thoughts as they considered what
move to make. From analyses of their verbal reports, de Groot
discovered that there was no association between skill level and the
number of moves ahead a player thought in advance of the current move.
Instead, he found evidence for a perceptual basis of chess expertise. As
de Groot put it, the grandmaster “immediately ‘sees’ the core of the
problem in the position” whereas the weaker player “finds it with
difficulty—or misses it completely” (p. 320). de Groot attributed this
ability to a “connoisseurship” (p. 321) that develops through years of
experience playing the game.
Nearly 30 years later, de Groot's (1946/1978) work was the inspiration for Chase and Simon's (1973a)
classic study of chess expertise, which marks the beginning of
cognitive psychologists' interest in expertise. Testing three chess
players—a master, an intermediate-level player, and a beginner—Chase and
Simon found that there was a positive relationship between chess skill
and memory for chess positions, but only when they were plausible game
positions. When the positions were random arrangements of pieces, there
was almost no effect of chess skill on memory. Based on these findings, Chase and Simon (1973b)
concluded that although “there clearly must be a set of specific
aptitudes...that together comprise a talent for chess, individual
differences in such aptitudes are largely overshadowed by immense
individual differences in chess experience. Hence, the overriding factor
in chess skill is practice” (p. 279).
The experts-are-made view has held sway in the scientific literature ever since. Over 20 years ago, in a pivotal article, Ericsson et al. (1993) proposed
that individual differences in performance in complex domains (music,
chess, sports, etc.) largely reflect differences in the amount of time
people have spent engaging in deliberate practice, which
“includes activities that have been specially designed to improve the
current level of performance” (p. 368). In the first of two studies,
Ericsson et al. recruited violinists from a Berlin music academy and
asked them to estimate the amount of hours per week they had devoted to
deliberate practice since taking up the violin. The “best” violinists
had accumulated an average of over 10,000 h of deliberate practice by
age 20, which was about 2500 h more than the average for the “good”
violinists and about 5000 h more than the average for the least
accomplished “teacher” group. In a second study, Ericsson et al. found
that “expert” pianists, who were selected to be similar in skill level
to the good violinists in the first study, had accumulated an average of
over 10,000 h of deliberate practice by age 20, compared to only about
2000 h for “amateur” pianists (see Ericsson, 2006; for further discussion of these results).
Ericsson et al. (1993)
concluded that “high levels of deliberate practice are necessary to
attain expert level performance” (p. 392). More controversially, they
added:
Our theoretical framework can also provide a sufficient account of the major facts about the nature and scarcity of exceptional performance. Our account does not depend on scarcity of innate ability (talent) and hence agrees better with the earlier reviewed findings of poor predictability of final performance by ability tests. We attribute the dramatic differences in performance between experts and amateurs-novices to similarly large differences in the recorded amounts of deliberate practice.Ericsson et al., (1993, p. 392), emphasis added
Ericsson
et al. further claimed that “individual differences in ultimate
performance can largely be accounted for by differential amounts of past
and current levels of practice” (p. 392), and stated:
We agree that expert performance is qualitatively different from normal performance and even that expert performers have characteristics and abilities that are qualitatively different from or at least outside the range of those of normal adults. However, we deny that these differences are immutable, that is, due to innate talent. Only a few exceptions, most notably height, are genetically prescribed. Instead, we argue that the differences between expert performers and normal adults reflect a life-long period of deliberate effort to improve performance in a specific domain.(p. 400)
Note to users:
Corrected proofs are Articles in Press that contain the authors' corrections. Final citation details, e.g., volume and/or issue number, publication year and page numbers, still need to be added and the text might change before final publication.
Although corrected proofs do not have all bibliographic details available yet, they can already be cited using the year of online publication and the DOI , as follows: author(s), article title, Publication (year), DOI. Please consult the journal's reference style for the exact appearance of these elements, abbreviation of journal names and use of punctuation.
When the final article is assigned to volumes/issues of the Publication, the Article in Press version will be removed and the final version will appear in the associated published volumes/issues of the Publication. The date the article was first made available online will be carried over.
Corrected proofs are Articles in Press that contain the authors' corrections. Final citation details, e.g., volume and/or issue number, publication year and page numbers, still need to be added and the text might change before final publication.
Although corrected proofs do not have all bibliographic details available yet, they can already be cited using the year of online publication and the DOI , as follows: author(s), article title, Publication (year), DOI. Please consult the journal's reference style for the exact appearance of these elements, abbreviation of journal names and use of punctuation.
When the final article is assigned to volumes/issues of the Publication, the Article in Press version will be removed and the final version will appear in the associated published volumes/issues of the Publication. The date the article was first made available online will be carried over.
No comments:
Post a Comment